Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 43
Filter
1.
Cogn Res Princ Implic ; 9(1): 29, 2024 05 12.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38735013

ABSTRACT

Auditory stimuli that are relevant to a listener have the potential to capture focal attention even when unattended, the listener's own name being a particularly effective stimulus. We report two experiments to test the attention-capturing potential of the listener's own name in normal speech and time-compressed speech. In Experiment 1, 39 participants were tested with a visual word categorization task with uncompressed spoken names as background auditory distractors. Participants' word categorization performance was slower when hearing their own name rather than other names, and in a final test, they were faster at detecting their own name than other names. Experiment 2 used the same task paradigm, but the auditory distractors were time-compressed names. Three compression levels were tested with 25 participants in each condition. Participants' word categorization performance was again slower when hearing their own name than when hearing other names; the slowing was strongest with slight compression and weakest with intense compression. Personally relevant time-compressed speech has the potential to capture attention, but the degree of capture depends on the level of compression. Attention capture by time-compressed speech has practical significance and provides partial evidence for the duplex-mechanism account of auditory distraction.


Subject(s)
Attention , Names , Speech Perception , Humans , Attention/physiology , Female , Male , Speech Perception/physiology , Adult , Young Adult , Speech/physiology , Reaction Time/physiology , Acoustic Stimulation
2.
Telemed J E Health ; 30(5): 1470-1478, 2024 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38215270

ABSTRACT

Background: Augmented reality head-worn displays (HWDs) may enable efficient remote support in the prehospital environment due to their hand-free operability, their "see-what-I-see" features, and their ability to superimpose digital content over the environment. Methods: In this simulation-based randomized controlled study, a remote mentor used either a phone or HWD to instruct 23 physicians on how to insert a Multi-Lumen Access Catheter into a mannequin. In the phone condition, information could be exchanged only verbally. In the HWD condition, the mentor could additionally see the participant's first-person view and show reference images. We hypothesized that participants who received instructions via the HWD would achieve better procedural performance (lower task completion times, fewer errors advancing the catheter) and exhibit different communication patterns than participants who received instructions via phone. Results: The HWD did not significantly reduce task completion times or errors during catheter advancement. However, by analyzing the frequency of communication events with a Poisson regression, we could demonstrate that with the HWD, the mentor had to request situation reports less often (p < 0.001) but provided more instructions (p = 0.004) and more feedback (p = 0.008). As a possible consequence, participants in the HWD condition rated their workload as lower than participants who used a phone to communicate (p = 0.45). Conclusion: The study demonstrates that HWD-based telemedicine systems can be rated positively by physicians, can benefit communication, and can provide more opportunities for the detection of clinical errors.


Subject(s)
Augmented Reality , Mentoring , Task Performance and Analysis , Humans , Male , Female , Mentoring/methods , Emergency Medical Services , Manikins , Adult , Communication , Clinical Competence , Simulation Training/methods
3.
Simul Healthc ; 2024 Jan 23.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38261519

ABSTRACT

INTRODUCTION: Prehospital teamwork occurs in dynamic environments where paramedics work together using technologies to care for patients. Despite increasing interest in using head-worn displays (HWDs) to support prehospital workers, little is known about how HWDs affect teamwork. METHODS: We tested the effect of HWDs on the team processes and patient care of paramedic trainee teams in a laboratory study using an online prehospital simulation environment, SPECTRa. In a randomized crossover design, 20 two-person teams worked in the SPECTRa laptop environment from separate physical rooms to assess and treat 2 simulated patients in 3 prehospital patient care scenarios. In each scenario, each trainee used either an HWD, a tablet computer (TAB), or no mobile device (CON) to help them monitor the vital signs of both patients. We measured team processes based around 3 themes of mutual understanding, team performance, and administered an 18-item questionnaire about teamwork and use of the devices. RESULTS: The mean number (HWD = 11; TAB = 7; P = 0.061) and duration (HWD = 1746 milliseconds; TAB = 1563 milliseconds; P = 0.504) of attention switches that teams made toward the mobile device did not differ with HWDs or TABs. However, teams switched attention between patients less with HWDs than with TABs (P = 0.026) or CON (P = 0.007) (medians: HWD = 5; TAB = 8; CON = 8). Teams communicated less when using HWDs than TABs (P = 0.017) (medians: HWD = 76; TAB = 96; CON = 83), but there were other mixed effects on communication. Team performance did not differ across device conditions on the timeliness to notice critical patient changes (P = 0.387) (medians: HWD = 244 seconds; TAB = 246 seconds; CON = 168 seconds) or to complete the scenarios (P = 0.212) (medians: HWD = 800 seconds; TAB = 913 seconds; CON = 835 seconds). Questionnaire results revealed some perceived benefits of the HWD. CONCLUSIONS: Head-worn displays may let prehospital teams monitor each other's performance more efficiently than TABs or CON, requiring less communication to maintain patient care performance with lower workload than with TABs. However, improvements in mutual understanding with HWDs compared with CON were more evident in teams' preferences than in actual behavior. Further research is needed to confirm and extend these results.

4.
Appl Ergon ; 112: 104072, 2023 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37327694

ABSTRACT

Spearcons are time-compressed speech phrases. When arranged in a sequence representing vital signs of multiple patients, spearcons may be more informative than conventional auditory alarms. However, multiple resource theory suggests that certain timeshared tasks might interfere with listeners' ability to understand spearcons. We tested the relative interference with spearcon identification from the following ongoing tasks: (1) manual tracking, (2) linguistic detection of spoken target words, (3) arithmetic true-false judgments, or (4) an ignored background speech control. Participants were 80 non-clinicians. The linguistic task worsened spearcon identification more than the tracking task, p < .001, and more than ignored background speech, p = .012. The arithmetic task worsened spearcon identification more than the tracking task, p < .001. The linguistic task and arithmetic task both worsened performance, p = .674. However, no ongoing task affected participants' ability to detect which patient(s) in a sequence had abnormal vital signs. Future research could investigate whether timeshared tasks affect non-speech auditory alerts.


Subject(s)
Clinical Alarms , Linguistics , Humans , Vital Signs , Monitoring, Physiologic , Speech
5.
Appl Ergon ; 107: 103918, 2023 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36395550

ABSTRACT

Standard pulse oximeter auditory tones do not clearly indicate departures from the target range of oxygen saturation (SpO2) of 90%-95% in preterm neonates. We tested whether acoustically enhanced tones would improve participants' ability to identify SpO2 range. Twenty-one clinicians and 23 non-clinicians used (1) standard pulse oximetry variable-pitch tones plus alarms; (2) beacon-enhanced tones without alarms in which reference tones were inserted before standard pulse tones when SpO2 was outside target range; and (3) tremolo-enhanced tones without alarms in which pulse tones were modified with tremolo when SpO2 was outside target range. For clinicians, range identification accuracies (mean (SD)) in the standard, beacon, and tremolo conditions were 52% (16%), 73% (14%) and 76% (13%) respectively, and for non-clinicians 49% (16%), 76% (13%) and 72% (14%) respectively, with enhanced conditions always significantly more accurate than standard. Acoustic enhancements to pulse oximetry clearly indicate departures from preterm neonates' target SpO2 range.


Subject(s)
Oximetry , Oxygen , Humans , Infant, Newborn , Acoustics , Heart Rate
7.
Resusc Plus ; 11: 100263, 2022 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35812718

ABSTRACT

Background: We tested principles that could lead to a future cognitive aid that offers an interpretation of the newborn's physiological state during resuscitation after birth. Using concordance among experts' interpretations of newborn vital sign patterns as an approximation for an algorithm that could provide an interpretation of the newborn's state, we explored the reliability and generalisability of experts' interpretations. Methods: Twelve neonatal experts viewed eight pairs of graphical trajectories showing newborns' heart rate and oxygen saturation records supplemented with differential diagnoses elicited previously from other experts. Each pair of trajectories included one trajectory on which the original differential diagnoses had been based, and a similar but novel trajectory to which the original differential diagnoses were now generalised. For each trajectory, experts ranked the differential diagnoses according to their likelihood. We calculated how similar the new experts' ranking was to the original experts' ranking for both original and novel trajectories. We used descriptive categories to interpret the strength of the similarity. Results: For the original and novel trajectories, the experts' rank ordering of differential diagnoses was mostly moderately to substantially similar to the original rank ordering by the original participants. There were mostly small differences in similarity scores between the paired original and novel trajectories; fewer than 25% of the participants suggested an alternative differential diagnosis. Conclusions: The concordance of experts' interpretations could serve as an approximation of the newborn's physiological state, and the interpretations could be generalised. The results may justify pursuing an algorithm to underpin a cognitive aid.

8.
Biomed Instrum Technol ; 56(2): 46-57, 2022 Apr 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35671368

ABSTRACT

Manufacturers could improve the pulse tones emitted by pulse oximeters to support more accurate identification of a patient's peripheral oxygen saturation (SpO2) range. In this article, we outline the strengths and limitations of the variable-pitch tone that represents SpO2 of each detected pulse, and we argue that enhancements to the tone to demarcate clinically relevant ranges are feasible and desirable. The variable-pitch tone is an appreciated and trusted feature of the pulse oximeter's user interface. However, studies show that it supports relative judgments of SpO2 trends over time and is less effective at supporting absolute judgments about the SpO2 number or conveying when SpO2 moves into clinically important ranges. We outline recent studies that tested whether acoustic enhancements to the current tone could convey clinically important ranges more directly, without necessarily using auditory alarms. The studies cover the use of enhanced variable-pitch pulse oximeter tones for neonatal and adult use. Compared with current tones, the characteristics of the enhanced tones represent improvements that are both clinically relevant and statistically significant. We outline the benefits of enhanced tones, as well as discuss constraints of which developers of enhanced tones should be aware if enhancements are to be successful.


Subject(s)
Oximetry , Oxygen , Adult , Humans , Infant, Newborn
9.
Hum Factors ; : 187208221075851, 2022 Apr 23.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35466744

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: In two experiments, we examined how quickly different visual alerts on a head-worn display (HWD) would capture participants' attention to a matrix of patient vital sign values, while multitasking. BACKGROUND: An HWD could help clinicians monitor multiple patients, regardless of where the clinician is located. We sought effective ways for HWDs to alert multitasking wearers to important events. METHODS: In two preclinical experiments, university student participants performed a visuomotor tracking task while simultaneously monitoring simulated patient vital signs on an HWD to detect abnormal values. Methods to attract attention to abnormal values included highlighting abnormal vital signs and imposing a white flash over the entire display. RESULTS: Experiment 1 found that participants detected abnormal values faster with high contrast than low contrast greyscale highlights, even while performing difficult tracking. In Experiment 2, a white flash of the entire screen quickly and reliably captured attention to vital signs, but less so on an HWD than on a conventional screen. CONCLUSION: Visual alerts on HWDs can direct users' attention to patient transition events (PTEs) even under high visual-perceptual load, but not as quickly as visual alerts on fixed displays. Aspects of the results have since been tested in a healthcare context. APPLICATION: Potential applications include informing the design of HWD interfaces for monitoring multiple processes and informing future research on capturing attention to HWDs.

10.
HERD ; 15(3): 375-394, 2022 07.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35437057

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVES: To (1) develop a simulation software environment to conduct prehospital research during the COVID-19 pandemic on paramedics' teamwork and use of mobile computing devices, and (2) establish its feasibility for use as a research and training tool. BACKGROUND: Simulation-based research and training for prehospital environments has typically used live simulation, with highly realistic equipment and technology-enhanced manikins. However, such simulations are expensive, difficult to replicate, and require facilitators and participants to be at the same location. Although virtual simulation tools exist for prehospital care, it is unclear how best to use them for research and training. METHODS: We present SPECTRa-Simulated Prehospital Emergency Care for Team Research-an online simulated prehospital environment that lets participants care concurrently for single or multiple patients remotely. Patient scenarios are designed using Laerdal's SimDesigner. SPECTRa records data about scenario states and participants' virtual interaction with the simulated patients. SPECTRa's supporting environment records participants' verbal communication and their visual and physical interactions with their interface and devices using Zoom conferencing and audiovisual recording. We discuss a pilot research implementation to assess SPECTRa's feasibility. RESULTS: SPECTRa allows researchers to systematically test small-team interaction in single- or multipatient care scenarios and assess the impact of mobile devices on participants' assessment and care of patients. SPECTRa also supports pedagogical features that could allow prehospital educators to provide individual trainees or teams with online simulation training and evaluation. CONCLUSIONS: SPECTRa, an online tool for simulating prehospital patient care, shows potential for remote healthcare research and training.


Subject(s)
COVID-19 , Emergency Medical Services , Humans , Manikins , Pandemics , Patient Care , Patient Care Team
11.
Ergonomics ; 65(2): 188-218, 2022 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34445922

ABSTRACT

Head-Worn Displays (HWD) can potentially support the mobile work of emergency responders, but it remains unclear whether teamwork is affected when emergency responders use HWDs. We reviewed studies that examined HWDs in emergency response contexts to evaluate the impact of HWDs on team performance and on team processes of situation awareness, communication, and coordination. Sixteen studies were identified through manual and systematic literature searches. HWDs appeared to improve the quality of team performance but they increased time to perform under some conditions; effects on team processes were mixed. We identify five challenges to explain the mixed results. We discuss four theoretical perspectives that might address the challenges and guide research needs-joint cognitive systems, distributed cognition, common ground, and dynamical systems. Researchers and designers should use process-based measures and apply greater theoretical guidance to uncover mechanisms by which HWDs shape team processes, and to understand the impact on team performance. Practitioner Summary: This review examines the effects of head-worn displays on teamwork performance and team processes for emergency response. Results are mixed, but study diversity challenges the search for underlying mechanisms. Guidance from perspectives such as joint cognitive systems, distributed cognition, common ground, and dynamical systems may advance knowledge in the area. Abbreviations: HWD: head-worn display; RC: remote collaboration; DD: data display; ARC: augmented remote collaboration; ACC: augmented collocated collaboration; SA: situation awareness; TSA: team situation awareness; CPR: cardiopulmonary resuscitation; SAGAT: situation awareness global assessment technique; SART: situation awareness rating technique.


Subject(s)
Awareness , Cognition , Communication , Data Display , Humans , Patient Care Team
12.
J Exp Psychol Appl ; 27(1): 69-83, 2021 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32915012

ABSTRACT

Auditory alarms in hospitals are ambiguous and do not provide enough information to support doctors and nurses' awareness of patient events. A potential alternative is the use of short segments of time-compressed speech, or spearcons. However, sometimes it might be desirable for patients to understand spearcons and sometimes not. We used reverse hierarchy theory to hypothesize that there will be a degree of compression where spearcons are intelligible for trained listeners but not for untrained listeners. In Experiment 1, spearcons were compressed to either 20% or 25% of their original duration. Their intelligibility was very high for trained participants, but also quite high for untrained participants. In Experiment 2 each word within each spearcon was compressed to a different degree based on the results of Experiment 1. This technique was effective in creating the desired difference in spearcon intelligibility between trained and untrained listeners. An implication of these results is that manipulating the degree of compression of spearcons "by word" can increase the effect of training so that untrained listeners reliably do not understand the content of the spearcons. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).


Subject(s)
Comprehension , Speech Perception , Humans , Speech
13.
Br J Anaesth ; 125(5): 826-834, 2020 11.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32682554

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: We compared anaesthetists' ability to identify haemoglobin oxygen saturation (SpO2) levels using two auditory displays: one based on a standard pulse oximeter display (varying pitch plus alarm) and the other enhanced with additional sound properties (varying pitch plus tremolo and acoustic brightness) to differentiate SpO2 ranges. METHODS: In a counter-balanced crossover study in a simulator, 20 experienced anaesthetists supervised a junior colleague (an actor) managing two airway surgery scenarios: once while using the enhanced auditory display and once while using a standard auditory display. Participants were distracted with other tasks such as paperwork and workplace interruptions, but were required to identify when SpO2 transitioned between pre-set ranges (target, low, critical) and when other vital signs transitioned out of a target range. They also identified the range once a transition had occurred. Visual displays were available for all monitored vital signs, but the numerical value for SpO2 was excluded. RESULTS: Participants were more accurate and faster at detecting transitions to and from the target SpO2 range when using the enhanced display (100.0%, 3.3 s) than when using the standard display plus alarm (73.2%, 27.4 s) (P<0.001 and P=0.004, respectively). They were also more accurate at identifying the SpO2 range once a transition had occurred when using the enhanced display (100.0%) than when using the standard display plus alarm (57.1%; P<0.001). CONCLUSIONS: The enhanced auditory display helps anaesthetists judge SpO2 levels more effectively than current auditory displays and may facilitate 'eyes-free' monitoring.


Subject(s)
Data Display , Oximetry/instrumentation , Acoustic Stimulation , Adult , Anesthesiologists , Clinical Alarms , Cross-Over Studies , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Operating Rooms/organization & administration , Oxygen/blood , Surveys and Questionnaires , Vital Signs
14.
Hum Factors ; 62(1): 124-137, 2020 02.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31180734

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: The pulse oximeter (PO) provides anesthesiologists with continuous visual and auditory information about a patient's oxygen saturation (SpO2). However, anesthesiologists' attention is often diverted from visual displays, and clinicians may inaccurately judge SpO2 values when relying on conventional PO auditory tones. We tested whether participants could identify SpO2 value (e.g., "97%") better with acoustic enhancements that identified three discrete clinical ranges by either changing abruptly at two threshold values (stepped-effects) or changing incrementally with each percentage value of SpO2 (smooth-effects). METHOD: In all, 79 nonclinicians participated in a between-subjects experiment that compared performance of participants using the stepped-effects display with those who used the smooth-effects display. In both conditions, participants heard sequences of 72 tones whose pitch directly correlated to SpO2 value, and whose value could change incrementally. Primary outcome was percentage of responses that correctly identified the absolute SpO2 percentage, ±1, of the last pulse tone in each sequence. RESULTS: Participants using the stepped-effects auditory tones identified absolute SpO2 percentage more accurately (M = 53.7%) than participants using the smooth-effects tones (M = 47.9%, p = .038). Identification of range and detection of transitions between ranges showed even stronger advantages for the stepped-effects display (p < .005). CONCLUSION: The stepped-effects display has more pronounced auditory cues at SpO2 range transitions, from which participants can better infer absolute SpO2 values. Further development of a smooth-effects display for this purpose is not necessary.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Auditory Perception , Clinical Alarms , Cues , Equipment Design , Monitoring, Physiologic , Oximetry , Oxygen Consumption , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Adult , Clinical Alarms/standards , Female , Humans , Male , Monitoring, Physiologic/instrumentation , Monitoring, Physiologic/standards , Oximetry/instrumentation , Oximetry/standards , Young Adult
15.
J Exp Psychol Appl ; 26(3): 522-537, 2020 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31763862

ABSTRACT

In this paper we describe the risks of complex applied research, especially in work domains where professional practitioners are scarce. For such research, careful preparation and piloting is needed, especially when estimating sample size is required for a full study. However, such pilot work may reduce the potential sample size for the full study. We describe how the these issues have been addressed in applied psychology contexts. We then present a case study illustrating how we determined sample size for a study investigating the impact of workplace interruptions on errors that intensive care unit nurses might make during medication preparation and administration. The pilot work was performed in a functionally related domain to nursing-bartending-and bartender participants filled cocktail orders. Pilot 1 investigated performance with 0 interruptions and applied a model from a field observation to estimate probable effect sizes and sample sizes with 1 or 4 interruptions per medication scenario. Pilot 2 collected empirical data on the effect of 1 or 4 interruptions per cocktail scenario on cocktail errors and estimated sample size for the medication study, which was subsequently successfully run. The applied community could benefit from further discussions about these issues and the means for addressing them. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2020 APA, all rights reserved).


Subject(s)
Critical Care Nursing , High Fidelity Simulation Training , Medication Errors/statistics & numerical data , Organizational Case Studies , Psychology, Applied , Female , Humans , Male , Pilot Projects , Workload
16.
Anesth Analg ; 129(4): 997-1004, 2019 10.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31283622

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: When engaged in visually demanding tasks, anesthesiologists depend on the auditory display of the pulse oximeter (PO) to provide information about patients' oxygen saturation (SpO2). Current auditory displays are not always effective at providing SpO2 information. In this laboratory study, clinician and nonclinician participants identified SpO2 parameters using either a standard auditory display or an auditory display enhanced with additional acoustic properties while performing distractor tasks and in the presence of background noise. METHODS: In a counterbalanced crossover design, specialist or trainee anesthesiologists (n = 25) and nonclinician participants (n = 28) identified SpO2 parameters using standard and enhanced PO auditory displays. Participants performed 2 distractor tasks: (1) arithmetic verification and (2) keyword detection. Simulated background operating room noise played throughout the experiment. Primary outcomes were accuracies to (1) detect transitions to and from an SpO2 target range and (2) identify SpO2 range (target, low, or critical). Secondary outcomes included participants' latency to detect target transitions, accuracy to identify absolute SpO2 values, accuracy and latency of distractor tasks, and subjective judgments about tasks. RESULTS: Participants were more accurate at detecting target transitions using the enhanced display (87%) than the standard display (57%; odds ratio, 7.3 [95% confidence interval {CI}, 4.4-12.3]; P < .001). Participants were also more accurate at identifying SpO2 range using the enhanced display (86%) than the standard display (76%; odds ratio, 2.7 [95% CI, 1.6-4.6]; P < .001). Secondary outcome analyses indicated that there were no differences in performance between clinicians and nonclinicians for target transition detection accuracy and latency, SpO2 range identification accuracy, or absolute SpO2 value identification. CONCLUSIONS: The enhanced auditory display supports more accurate detection of target transitions and identification of SpO2 range for both clinicians and nonclinicians. Despite their previous experience using PO auditory displays, clinicians in this laboratory study were no more accurate in any SpO2 outcomes than nonclinician participants.


Subject(s)
Acoustics/instrumentation , Anesthesiologists/psychology , Auditory Perception , Clinical Alarms , Oximetry/instrumentation , Oxygen/blood , Acoustic Stimulation , Adult , Attention , Biomarkers/blood , Clinical Competence , Cross-Over Studies , Female , Humans , Male , Noise/adverse effects , Perceptual Masking , Signal Detection, Psychological , Time Factors
17.
J Exp Psychol Appl ; 25(4): 647-673, 2019 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30883150

ABSTRACT

Spearcons-time-compressed speech phrases-may be an effective way of communicating vital signs to clinicians without disturbing patients and their families. Four experiments tested the effectiveness of spearcons for conveying oxygen saturation (SpO2) and heart rate (HR) of one or more patients. Experiment 1 demonstrated that spearcons were more effective than earcons (abstract auditory motifs) at conveying clinical ranges. Experiment 2 demonstrated that casual listeners could not learn to decipher the spearcons whereas listeners told the exact vocabulary could. Experiment 3 demonstrated that participants could interpret sequences of sounds representing multiple patients better with spearcons than with pitch-based earcons, especially when tones replaced the spearcons for normal patients. Experiment 4 compared multiple-patient monitoring of two vital signs with either spearcons, a visual display showing SpO2 and HR in the same temporal sequence as the spearcons, or a visual display showing multiple patient levels simultaneously. All displays conveyed which patients were abnormal with high accuracy. Visual displays better conveyed the vital sign levels for each patient, but cannot be used eyes-free. All displays showed accuracy decrements with working memory load. Spearcons may be viable for single and multiple patient monitoring. Further research should test spearcons with more vital signs, during multitasking, and longitudinally. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2019 APA, all rights reserved).


Subject(s)
Auditory Perception/physiology , Clinical Alarms , Heart Rate/physiology , Monitoring, Physiologic , Oxygen Consumption/physiology , Speech Perception/physiology , Speech , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Surveys and Questionnaires , Young Adult
18.
J Clin Monit Comput ; 33(6): 1119-1127, 2019 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30721389

ABSTRACT

PURPOSE: Supervising anesthesiologists overseeing several operating rooms must be aware of the status of multiple patients, so they can consult with the anesthetist in single operating rooms or respond quickly to critical events. However, maintaining good situation awareness can be challenging when away from patient bedsides or a central monitoring station. In this proof-of-concept study, we evaluated the potential of a head-worn display that showed multiple patients' vital signs and alarms to improve supervising anesthesiologists' situation awareness. METHODS: Eight supervising anesthesiologists each monitored the vital signs of patients in six operating rooms for 3 h with the head-worn display, and for another 3 h without the head-worn display. In interviews with each anesthesiologist, we assessed in which situations the head-worn display was used and whether the continuous availability of the vital signs improved situation awareness. We also measured situation awareness quantitatively from six of the eight anesthesiologists, by instructing them to press a button whenever they noticed a patient alarm. RESULTS: The median number of patient alarms occurring was similar when the anesthesiologists monitored with the head-worn display (42.0) and without the head-worn display (40.5). However, the anesthesiologists noticed significantly more patient alarms with the head-worn display (66.7%) than without (7.1%), P = 0.028, and they reported improved situation awareness with the head-worn display. The head-worn display helped the anesthesiologists to perceive and comprehend patients' current status and to anticipate future developments. A negative effect of the head-worn display was its tendency to distract during demanding procedures. CONCLUSIONS: Head-worn displays can improve supervising anesthesiologists' situation awareness in multiple-patient monitoring situations. The anesthesiologists who participated in the study expressed enthusiasm about monitoring patients with a head-worn display and wished to use and evaluate it further.


Subject(s)
Anesthesiologists , Anesthesiology/methods , Awareness , Monitoring, Intraoperative/instrumentation , Adult , Cross-Over Studies , Data Display , Female , Germany , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Monitoring, Intraoperative/methods , Monitoring, Physiologic/methods , Operating Rooms , Physicians , Reproducibility of Results , User-Computer Interface , Vital Signs
19.
Hum Factors ; 61(2): 288-304, 2019 03.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30260675

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: The aim was to compare the effectiveness of two auditory displays, implemented with spearcons (time-compressed speech), for monitoring multiple patients. BACKGROUND: Sequences of sounds can convey information about patients' vital signs, such as oxygen saturation (SpO2) and heart rate (HR). We tested whether participants could monitor five patients using spearcon-based sound sequences. METHOD: A 2 × 3 within-subjects design was used. The first factor was interface, with two levels: the ALL interface used spearcons to convey vital signs for all five patients, whereas the ABN (abnormal) interface represented patients who had normal vital signs with a low-pitched single-tone sound and patients who had at least one abnormal vital sign with spearcons. The second factor was the number of patients who had at least one abnormal vital sign: there were one, two, or three such patients in each monitoring sequence. Participants were 40 nonclinicians. RESULTS: Participants identified abnormal patients' SpO2 and HR levels and located abnormal patients in the sound sequence more accurately with the ABN interface than the ALL interface. Accuracy declined as the number of abnormal patients increased. Participants associated ABN with easier identification of vital signs, resulting in higher ratings of confidence and pleasantness compared with ALL. CONCLUSION: Sequences of spearcons may support effective eyes-free monitoring of multiple patients. APPLICATION: Sequences of spearcons may be useful in monitoring multiple patients and the underlying design principles may extend to monitoring in other domains such as industrial process control or control of multiple autonomous vehicles.


Subject(s)
Clinical Alarms , Data Display , Equipment Design , Monitoring, Physiologic/instrumentation , Sound , Speech , Vital Signs , Adolescent , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Young Adult
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...